New Movements
                     in the Study and
                    Teaching of Biology



 




                                Edited by Cyril Selmes




                               Temple Smith - London
                                                 1974


Hugh Iltis is Professor of Botany and Director of the
Herbarium at the University of Wisconsin in Madison.
His special interests are in problems of plant distribution
and evolution, the origin of maize, and man's optimum
environment and need for nature. He has led botanical
expeditions to Costa Rica, Mexico and Peru and has
published widely in scientific and popular journals. Much of
his time recently has been spent in lecturing on the urgent
need for the preservation of biotic communities, and
criticizing agricultural practices, especially the 'Green
Revolution'.

Acknowledgements

     Professor Iltis wishes to thank George B. Van Schaack,
Dan McKinley, Tim Clark, Marylin Bagley, Ralph Buchsbaum,
Fred Iltis and Joyce Gutstein for valuable comments.
New Movements in the Study and Teaching of
Biology.  Edited by Cyril Selmes, Temple
Smith, London.  1974. pp. 289-317.

           Flowers and human ecology

                                                        HUGH ILTIS

A love of diversity
Why do I get an irrepressible urge to defend what I love, the
beauty and diversity of nature, and, especially the disarming
loveliness of its flowers?
   One of my earliest recollections is joyfully picking huge
and wildly unorganized bouquets of flowers on a Moravian
mountain meadow, scabiosas, bluebells and daisies, and then
lying on my back in a 'nest' surrounded by tall, tall grass
watching the bees and the clouds. Ever since then, I have
been an addicted botanist, 'half-plant', as an old friend of
mine used to describe me.
   My family was always interested in natural history and
that tradition gave me much botanical stimulation and often
a rather pointed direction to become a botanist (sometimes
whether I liked it or not!). Besides being a professional
botanist, and an advocate of adult education, my father was
also a preservationist already in the 1920s; in a simplistic
way, he tried to preserve an acre here and there for its rare
flora. Way ahead of his time, he was singularly unsucessful.
Then hardly anybody thought preservation worth while, not
even the Socialists, among whose ranks he was active, for the
archaic creed that the 'people's' needs must always come
first, no matter what, was then as now an often ill-applied
battle cry against injustice.
   In any case, by the time I went to the University of
Tennessee in Knoxville I was well prepared to appreciate
Professor Jack Sharp, a former student of the still active
environmental pioneer Paul Sears. In 1945, Sharp botanically
explored the Mexican Sierra Madre and returned with grim
tales of horrendous erosion, increasing overpopulation, and
outright destruction of forests. Despairing, but never silent,
he kept hammering at the issues, at the blind insanity of both
hungry and greedy men in a world spinning out of control.


290
But the nearby Great Smoky Mountains National Park
offered an alternative lesson - nature can be preserved for its
own sake, remain unharvested, and still serve man most
genuinely. It was obvious we needed to preserve more
national parks, more wild land.
    Four years at the Missouri Botanical Garden in St. Louis
provided my professional training. There were trips to the
Ozarks with botanist Julian Steyermark whose early voice
argued for preservation of rare habitats. There were classes
with Edgar Anderson who, while a fabulously gifted botany
teacher, never took any real interest in preservation. This
kept bothering me, for his lack of concern was the rule
among botanists there, rather than the exception. It
seemed that many, perhaps most, and even the most
excellent, professional biologists felt no social responsibilities
towards nature, or even towards the very organisms they
busily studied. I had one or two teachers who could not care
less and said so. The less they cared, the more I wondered
what sense there was in revising a genus of plants when it
would soon become extinct. And what a pity not to save that
which is so beautiful and gives so much joy.
   Like all graduate students at the MBG, I took a trip to the
tropics with a fellow student. We went to Costa Rica, where
we wondered at the tropical flora in more ways than one.
Behind the Instituto de Agricola Inter-americana in Turrialba,
along the Rio Reventazon, there was a small protected patch
of virgin tropical rain forest. It contained what still is probably
the largest population in the world of a most remarkable giant
tree, Oreomunnea (Engelhardia) pterocarpa, of the walnut
family, representing a group of 'living fossils' with an excellent
seventy million-year-long paleontological record. Of this
species, there exist only a few hundred trees in the world,
and about fifty were here in this little patch of timber, this
sylvan cathedral, their crowns reaching to the sky. What a
marvel! Yet several of the more 'practical' , forestry-
oriented botanists at Turrialba thought this forest, and with
it these rare trees, ought to be cut. Thus, not only were some
botanists not preservationists, some were even professional
environmental rapists. That thought alone still makes me
sweat! How senseless, how blind can men be? Surely, if only
fifty of anything are left, they demand protection. Thus,
                                                                                          291
slowly, slowly but surely, my childhood love and aesthetic
awareness of nature matured into a passionate desire to save
it.
 
The preservation of nature as a passionate concern
When I came to the University of Wisconsin in 1955, I was
flung head-on into an old well-established preservation
tradition, a new way of thinking about the landscape and its
preservation. There I found organizations called 'The Friends
of the Native Landscape' and 'Citizens Natural Resource
Association', as well as many concerned new friends and
many new books, such as Aldo Leopold's A Sand County
Almanac. They taught me to open emotional and intellectual
doors previously kept shut by timid 'hang-ups' about the
seeming sentimentalities of nature preservation, by feelings of
embarassment, which our carefully orchestrated culture,
based on nature's destruction, ensures we all have in
abundance. In Wisconsin, a state board for preservation of
scientific areas administered choice scientific areas preserved
solely for teaching and research. Its members, including the
famous ecologist J. T. Curtis, supported a new concept: that
the state, the body politic, is ultimately responsible for the
preservation of nature; that to preserve flowers you have first
to preserve and manage their total habitat; and that, if you
do not thus ritualise preservation and sanctify it into law,
there can be no hope for fish, fowl, or flower!
   Not only the state, the sum total of all its citizens, but all
citizens themselves must be personally involved in the
preservation of nature. But that brought up some very
difficult questions. Who will pay for this? What if a poor
farmer owns a magnificent virgin oak-maple-walnut forest on
part of his land? Is it his duty to save it, voluntarily and
without recompense, for university research? Is it his
responsibility to keep this ecosystem intact, and as a
conseqence deprive his family and sacrifice his profit for the
long-range good of posterity, or for some well-to-do city folk
to enjoy the wild flowers? What had posterity, or the wild
flower lovers, ever done for the farmer? Clearly, the fate of
nature cannot be left to the farmer's altruism. There are some
who would perhaps wish it to be so. Yet this is quite


292
unrealistic. Rather, preservation of nature and of diversity
must be considered the duty and reponsibility of the state
because it benefits all citizens. It must, therefore, be paid
out of public funds and administered as a governmental
function; its preservation cannot be left to chance or to the
goodwill of a farmer. If the state is unwilling, or as yet
unenlightened enough, to purchase such forests or prairies,
then private citizens should organize and buy the land.
   Nature preservation was often presented with promises of
economic reward as the main rationale: the foods yet to be
discovered, the drugs yet to be found, the lovely shrub or
bulb yet to be brought into cultivation, the breeding stock to
be saved - nothing useful to us or our children should be
allowed to become extinct. The utility of nature preser-
vation, especially of scientific areas, was a main theme of
many a lecture (Iltis 1959). Despite its flaws, this still makes
much sense, and, to many people, remains the only valid
argument.
   However, basing preservation on such purely utilitarian
views left out any genuinely natural reasons. Gradually, some
rather subtle but very basic questions kept intruding - what if
people simply required unspoiled nature with its patterns and
diversities to be happy, or simply to  maintain their health?
Did we not evolve within nature? Are we not adapted to it?
   In 1962, the environmental crisis broke, suddenly, with
Silent Spring. Even while Rachel Carson was publicly
ridiculed as a sentimental old lady, and the ecological
concern over DDT as silly, the subtle necrotic effects of
pollution became only too apparent. It began to look as if
DDT and mercury compounds might inherit what was left
over after cow and plough. In the meanwhile, from colleges
of agriculture to big chemical corporations, the organized
opposition to any sensible approach was making sure that the
public would remain deluded and misinformed. Profit was
profit - to hell with nature.

More primitive cultures- more natural ways -
more tranquil humans
During this time, two trips to Latin America stimulated a
major turning point in my thinking. One, in 1960, took us to
Mexico to search for a natural and quickly degraded
                                                                                      293
insecticide
, the 'Sabadillo' (Schoenocaulon) of the lily
family, and another, in 1962, to Peru to study the evolution
and taxonomy of wild and weedy potatoes. Travelling
through primitive villages and ancient towns we saw Indians
in a new light. In the high Andes or Sierras especially, the
natives lived with the land, in it and from it, and, poor as
they were, only rarely deliberately destroyed it. Perhaps this
was merely natural, because they did not have tractors or
other technological weapons and could scarcely be violent
with it. In any case, uneducated, even illiterate as they were,
the Indian men and women knew a great deal about both
wild and cultivated crops, where they grew best, what they
were good for, and which they loved best for taste, colour or
form. They knew by common name hundreds of wild species
of plants; and each of even the most subtly distinguished
'criollo' varieties of potatoes, for example, had its own
common name. The phrases es bonito or es linda, often
applied to a flower or to a brilliantly coloured ear of corn,
transferred to us their gentle appreciation of natural beauty.
   They were also gentle and undemanding of their children
as well. I watched women breast-feed their young children as
evolution had meant that relationship to function, on
roadsides, on buses, in a calmness and naturalness quite rare
(at best) in hectic suburban America. There were no plastic
bottles with canned formulas here, only an innate, quiet
understanding of the nurselings' needs and the undemanding
relaxed love of their mothers (cf. Montagu 1972). These
babies were suckled for two or even three years, and if this
causes surprise, it is well to remember that our closest kin,
the chimpanzee, may suckle its young to the age of four!
   The contrast between that blessed peace and our frantic
turmoil kept obsessing my brain. In the villages, even older
babies were always carried, usually by their mothers, or by
older brothers or sisters. They hardly ever cried, which one
could hardly say for offspring in urban America. When they
were hungry. they fed, when sleepy, they slept. Some
questions came back again and again: if, in their natural way,
these mothers were fulfilling their young children's natural,
innate demands, was that the ultimate reason for the
tranquility of the adults? If babies have to have mothers to
nurse from, and mothers to be loved by in order to be happy



294
and tranquil, may not humanity similarly require nature to
live in, and nature to interact with, to be happy and tranquil?
For us, after all, nature should know best: did we not evolve
to fit her ecological requirements; or better, were not our
ecological requirements determined by her? These analogies
may be somewhat misleading. Nature does not always love
us, by any means. Yet they do allow us to ask a very basic
question: what is it, in nature, that man needs? That makes
him happy?

Is there a basic optimum human environment?
The insight hit hard one day, that love for and involvement
with nature, as a human trait nearly universal among people
both primitive and civilised, may in part be genetically
determined; that human needs for love, must be inherent (Iltis,
1966). Man's love for natural colours, patterns, and har-
monies, his preference for forest-grassland ecotones which he
recreates wherever he settles, even in drastically different
landscapes, must be the result (at least to a very large degree)
of Darwinian natural selection through eons of mammilian
and anthropoid evolutionary time and even through the ten
millenia of human agricultural history. Out eyes and ears,
noses, brains, and bodies have all been shaped by nature.
Would it not then be incredible indeed, if savannas and forest
groves, flowers and animals, the multiplicity of environ-
mental components to which our bodies were originally
shaped, were not, at the very least, still important to us?
Would not such a concept of 'nature' be a major part of what
might be called a basic optimum human environment?
   One could elaborate this insight in a thousand obvious as
well as more subtle ways. Yes, air to breathe, water to drink,
plants and animals to hunt and eat, sounds to hear, images to
see, textures to touch, land to wander on, people to love.
But, moreover, does not spring, with sprouting meadows and
flowering trees, stimulate joy? Do not then lovers go courting
in the country, flowers in hand, sensitive as never before to
the 'feel' of the awakening, growing land, to every odour of
blossum or lover, intoxicated with the dark, demonic urges to
dance, to touch, to mate close to the earth? And do not boys
and girls fish, men hunt and fight, both men and women
                                                                                            295
garden? Do we not, if we have money, build swimming pools
or greenhouses, or second homes in the mountains? Do we
not stream out of our cities by the million to camp in nature
every spring and summer, a weekend exodus of human
lemmings? Do we not, if we have a choice, keep pet dogs,
cats, or fish, cyclamen, primula, or begonia, and humidifiers
and flickering fireplaces in our miserably cold northern
winters as ethnological fragments, almost symbolic bits, of
that ancestral ecosystem which shaped our physical needs
and subconscious desires? Could this also be the reason why
some of us get seduced by surrogate plastic flowers, or even
plastic trees (Krieger, 1973) ? Is it to satisfy such basic innate
needs that we wish to keep in touch with nature?
     If there is then a biologically valid concept of a basic
optimum environment for mankind, would it not have to
include, because of the innate needs of the original naked
apes and ape men, not only mothers for babies, but much
real nature for children and adults? Is it not this optimum
human environment, modified somewhat, to be sure, during
hominoid evolution, definable in its essence as a compromise
containing maximum contact with nature without giving up
all the advantages of culture and civilization? And would not
human needs for this nature-contact be of different intensity
and quality for different ontogenetic stages: water, mud and
love for babies, shrubs to hide behind and protected
excitement for six-year-olds, groups to defend, fields to dig in
and grass for adolescents and adults? How long
since we left the savannas? the tribes? the villages? Surely not
long enough for all of us to have lost all need for their natural
settings?
    Here, finally, was an argument for nature preservation free
of purely utilitarian considerations: not just clean air because
polluted air gives cancer; not just pure water because polluted
water kills the fish we might like to catch; not just saving
plants or ducks because they could be 'useful' or 'edible'; but
preservation of the natural ecosystem to give body and soul a
chance to function in the way they were selected to function
in their original phylogenetic home. The ultimate argument
for nature preservation, as well as for landscape architecture
or urban planning, would then rest squarely on evolutionary
principles.



296
   The optimum environment of any organism is the one in
which it evolved, whether man, grasshopper, water lily or
amoeba. This is given - it has to be, and no one has to prove
it. On the other hand, most of us city dwellers live in a
human environment which is but a few thousand, and to
most human genotypes, only about a hundred years old. In
1810, only about 8 percent of the world population lived in
towns; today it approaches 50 per cent. Today, many of
these people live in metropolises which, in many crucial
parameters, are not optimum environments by any stretch of
the imagination. And why not? Because, in them, we are like
wild colts which evolved to roam the steppes but are tethered
in a dark cave. Unlike rapid cultural evolution, physiological
and physical evolution is an immensely slow process in which
a hundred and sixty years (or eight sexual generations) will
produce no, or at best only the most minute and imper-
ceptible, changes in our basic adaptations. True, we must
have slowly evolved into more docile creatures during the
10,000 years of predominantly agricultural life, and today,
especially since the industrial-urban revolution, we are still
evolving, perhaps even rapidly, in response to urban selection
pressures, but whither and to what final fate no one, of
course, can say. While perhaps in 20,000 years we may evolve
into a half-blind, half-deaf Homo Sapiens subspecies, post-
sapiens, composed of environmental morons programmed to
tolerate pollution, noise and ugliness, and perhaps allergic to
the confusing diversity of natural conditions, today we are
still part of Homo s. sapiens, a subspecies with strong genetic
needs for the natural environment, whose members, depend-
ing on individual variability, try at every turn to find nature
surrogates, from air-conditioned humidified domiciles, to
aquaria with fish, to plastic trees designed by well-meaning
landscape architects.
   Clearly, the finest human and artistic attributes of even the
most civilised man and culture have their roots in nature.
Judging from the intensity of man's feelings and his search
for substitutes we may yet preserve these roots and their
substrate as the basis of his humanity. The substitutes we
find for our original habitats are legion, and many are
important and satisfying. Clearly, we cannot all go back to
the African savannas, for there are too many of us! And even
                                                                                        297
if we could we would not want to - for most of us may have
lost the ability to live and hunt like cavemen. Yet if,
depending on age, climate, and season, we wish to experience
a highly optimal physiological-psychological environment, we
must retain the option of replicating the environment of our
ancestors thousands and tens of thousands of years ago, at
least now and then.  And that means a return to biotic
diversity and adventure, reaching from the simplicity of a
colourful flower garden all the way to the unmanipulated,
undepleted complexity of a wilderness. And, above all, it
means the opportunity for intimate sensory contact with
natural beauty, both cultivated and wild. We do need
well-kept colourful gardens, as we do fields of grain and fibre.
But to me at least, in terms of sensory contact, these are only
special substitutes for wild land, with crucial yet limited
functions and usually with low diversity.
    Thus, today, for the sake of diversity, we have ever more
urgent reasons to preserve the many, many species of plants
and animals which cannot be grown in gardens or maintained
and reproduced in zoos. Except for a few domesticated
species, this can only be done by protecting their original
wild habitat, by preserving the very special ecosystem ro
which each species is adapted and of which it is a part. (Even
for man, despite his immense adaptability, only time will tell
how strictly that great principle holds true for him as well.)
This is why we need so many nature preserves, whether small
scientific areas and county parks or giant wilderness areas of
immense size, because there are so many species and
ecosystems to be protected ! This is why we need inter-
national agreements on animals and ecosystems, subsidies and
concerted efforts for the preservation of the tropics and the
oceans on a scale as yet hard to imagine. And for the
preservation of many habitats even tomorrow may be too
late. Thus, and finally, this is why every biology teacher must
be passionately committed to the establishment and pro-
tection of biotic preserves of whatever size. Not only
membership in preservation societies is required of the
teacher, both to help financially and personally in the task
and to give every student a shining example of genuine
committment, but the biology teacher must transfer to the
student tne respect, the love, and the value such preserved



298
and undeveloped areas have to society. He must in fact teach
the student how to use them and protect them.
   In arguing for preservation of nature, the diversity of
wildernes is not the only concern today. We need to preserve
human ecosystems showing high diversity, stability and
natural beauty. Thus, apple or peach orchards, vegetable
plots and fields of specialty crops such as artichokes, or
small low-yielding but diverse farms on the outskirts of cities
are all agricultural communities, essentially ecotonal in
character, combining forest and grassland into a kind of
anthropogenic savanna of great aesthetic and ecological value
for many people, Such small farms with many kinds of crops
and groves of trees are quite stable, unlike the monocultural
systems of Montana wheat or Iowa corn. The picturesque
landscapes of Normandy, or of my native Czechoslovakia, the
alternating farms and forests of southern Ohio, the mixed
farming area of the Ontario Peninsula, and the unique
combination of farming, market gardening, woodland and
lowland in Kent and Sussex also deserve our concern. Now
much of such land is being deliberately subdivided in the
name of growth and profit or developed and destroyed in the
name of progress in a global gesture of supreme carelessness.
Biology teachers and planners, especially, need to explain to
their students the elusive meaning of this loss to man and
offer them viable alternatives: statuatory limits to the growth
of suburbia, the benefits of good zoning, with changes in
taxation structure to allow farming next to apartment
houses, and above all a deliberate policy of restraint on
growth and on making money out of the selling of land.

What new research would help humanity
construct a more liveable world?
The general malaise of mankind, especially of Western
civilization, would make it appear that urban humanity has
become the unhappy involuntary guinea-pig of a giant
experiment in sensory deprivation (coupled to lopsided
sensory overstimulation), in which the crowded cities are our
cages, and the inexorably cumulative acceleration of tech-
nology and the loss of nature the experimental variables.
Nobody really is exclusively to blame - not technology per se,
not capitalism, nor overpopulation. But the syndrome is such
                                                                                      299
that nobody can get out of participating in this experiment
(except temporarily, perhaps, the young drop-outs that pack
a tent and go back to the woods). True, while we have
reached much higher standards of living and a longer life
expectancy, there are other basic measures of an optimum
environment, measures which would make it appear that we
are reaping the synergistic effects of this sensory deprivation-
overstimulation syndrome on a gigantic scale. The sea of
broken homes; the epidemic of first unwanted, then unloved,
then battered children; the ill-disguised anger of many
childless and husbandless women seeking surrogate solutions
for their biologically unfulfilled lives from Cosmopolitan; the
blandness of purposeless males seeking salvation in Playboy
philosophies; and the increasing alienation of young and old
alike, both from society and from nature, all speak of
unpredicted, unwanted consequences of our high culture, if
not of an inescapable flood-tide of social pathology, and
certainly not of an optimum environment to which humanity
is genetically adapted.
   Explanation for this gradual collapse of social structure
and biological intercoherence (of man to nature, including
other men) are legion, and include crowding, pollution, and
the unbearable continuous roaring noises of the large city.
Significantly, recent research has shown mental disease to be
twice as high in New York City than in rural northern
Illinois, a not unexpected conclusion. Yet why should this
be? If we even cursorily analyse the difference between these
two habitats of man, New York City and northern rural
Illinois, we find that in addition to the lack of noise,
pollution, and crowding, there is the presence of beautiful
green plants, which to us mammals have always been either
'neutral' and unthreatening or even inspiring, as well as to a
variety of animals, all combined into an infinitely varied
landscape.  Could it then be that the stimuli of non-human
living diversity makes the difference between sanity and
madness ? If so, is it not about time to find out why? Is it not
finally time for experimental psychologists, human etho-
logists, and landscape architects or urban planners to get
together and design experiments, scientifically indisputable
and clinically impeccable, which would give us insights into
how the brain of the human animal reacts to natural diversity



300
and quality (beauty), to technological sterility, and what
exactly happens when we look at the symmetrical pattern of
a leaf or smell the odour of a brilliant flower? With modern
EEG equipment and a little imagination, it would be rather
easy to design experiments, using a diversity of groups of
different ages and with diverse social and ecological back-
grounds, comparing children with adults, farm youngsters
with high rise apartment dwellers, the emotionally healthy
and the mentally sick. Hardly anything is known about the
way we neurologically relate to our surroundings. Could it
be, perhaps, that we don't want to know? That if the truth
were out, many of the powers that be would then be forced
to stop making a desert out of the green earth, which would
cut into profits, among other things, and rising GNPs?
   The truth of the matter is that hardly anyone has bothered
to think about the connection between the alarm of the
'preservationists', the social pathology of the cities, and the
overwhelming ignorance regarding human needs in general
and especially for the aesthetics of nature, a subject generally
reserved for the abiological disciplines of the humanities. It is
thus interesting to read at the conclusion of Reflections on
Environmental Education (Stillman, 1972) a very stimula-
ting, if often rather derogatory and ill-informed attack, on
'prophets of doom' (such as this author), the following
perceptive admission:

   I have been able to find nothing in the environmental
   literature to help me understand what it is in natural
   environments that inspires human imagination, response,
   and excitement. Whatever it is, it should be added to the
   tool-bag of sensitive and imaginative teachers.

Indeed it should! For 'whatever' Stillman is talking about
is our innate response to the natural environment, a response
selected (like us) over many millions of years. It is a natural
human response to our original basic optimum environment,
to the world of nature. There have indeed been no efforts
made whatever to establish in a clinical and scientific way
just how this interrelationship functions. Yet, the above
insight is not all that ingenious. Wise men since time
immemorial have subjectively sensed the important relation-
ship of human happiness to natural conditions and incor-
                                                                                       301
porated it into ethics and religion. And artists have had it in
their bones since the beginnings of recorded cultural time in
the torchlit caves of Lascaux. Most are itself is but a
reflection of nature, caught by ear or eye, and transfixed by
the mirror of the human psyche. 'In a magnificent, but
seldom read essay, Susanne Langer (1967) has suggested that
all art is rooted in our response to natural form' (Stillman
1972). Others, from Goethe to our times, have agreed.
   But today it is not enough to pay homage to human love
for nature, nor to art's dependence on nature, but rather high
time to press for experimental proof and scientific under-
standing of the 'why' and the 'how', and thus furnish the
factual scientific-mechanistic bases and arguments for the
deliberate preservation of the natural environment.

Crowded rats, growing brains, and
denatured environments
Where are some of the frontiers in physiological research to
redirect man's view of himself? What does this have to do
with the biology teacher?
    Human needs for nature surely cannot be disputed.
Because we evolve in it, this, the biosphere, is the best of all
possible worlds for us; and if we lose it, or even lose pieces of
it, no one can predict the consequences. These cannot, in the
long run, be good. Just to look at our urban problems is to
glimpse into a hellish future darkly!
   It is common knowledge that denatured human environ-
ments produce denatured (i. e. unnatural) humans. This is
demonstrated over and over again in our crowded inner cities,
our human zoos (see Morris, 1969), and especially well in the
ultimate of denatured horrors invented by man - our sterile,
crowded prisons. It is now equally well-established scientific
knowledge that an unnatural rat environment produces
unnatural (i. e. sick) rats (cf. Ward, 1972). In a series of
classical experiments, John Calhoun of the National Institute
of Mental Health showed how increased crowding in rat
populations elicits maltreatment or abandonment of their
young, as well as an increase in homosexuality and irrational
violence, phenonmena which are replicated by crowded Homo
sapiens every day in the treeless slums of Chicago or under the
smoggy skies of Los Angeles, and in a lot of nicer places, too.



302
   The effects of crowding per se in man, irrespective of the
nature of the environment, have not been as well understood
or objectively studied. Recent tests (Ehrlich and Freedman
1971) suggest that under crowded conditions 'men become
more competetive, somewhat more severe, and like each other
less, whereas women become more cooperative and lenient
and like each other more', an ethologically not unexpected
finding. However, another study (Freedman, et al., 1971)
showed that people-density per se had no significant effect
on performance of a variety of specific tasks carried out
during four-hour periods on three consecutive days.
   It may thus well be, as many technological optimists
defending larger populations have hopefully repeated, that
crowding alone, by and in itself, isolated from any con-
comitant side effects, exerts no particularly bad sociological
influence on most people. (I personally doubt this, but for
the sake of argument we may accept it here.) Why, then, are
there so many of the most terrible modern human problems
endemic to the crowded urban centres? If the cause is not
crowding directly, perhaps it is crowding indirectly.  Could it
be, perhaps, that intense crowding, inevitably resulting in the
total destruction of all complex biological ecosystems
(allowing survival of a few species only, such as bacteria,
fungi, certain weeds, insects, sparrows and rats) indirectly
deprives man of a whole battery of inherited needs which can
be satisfied easily and well only by nature? To put it in
another way, since crowded places tend to be denatured,
would this indeed no seem to, at least indirectly, support the
ideas presented here: namely, that humans cannot live very
well without the biological diversity of nature, and especially
without the beauty and pattern of plants - to view, to
explore, to be stimulated by. If it is nature and its diversity
that has to be made available for man, the implications of
this concept to biology teaching, to landscape architecture,
urban planning, medicine, and to certain businesses, especially
flori-culture and tree nurseries, are vast. A fundamental and
synthesising, consciousness-raising re-evaluation of much of
our literature is clearly in order. (cf. McHarg's 1969 maps of
human pathologies concentrated in the heart of Philadelphia;
see also Tinbergen and Tinbergen, 1972, a most remarkable
ethological appraisal of autistic children, whree, while the

                                                                                         303
human interaction component is stressed, the natural needs
for health care are, if only barely, hinted at. Yet, one cannot help
but wonder whether such overly timid children might not
respond more readily to therapy in the uncrowded, unhur-
ried, non-intimidating and more natural - for children
especially - rural settings freely provided by village, farm or
camp.
   That 'poor', 'unstimulating', 'loveless' environments
'deprive' children, even without crowding, is likewise
common knowledge, as do environments without the stimula-
tion of some adversity, some rigour. But why should this be?
How does this work? If we could demonstrate what is
actually happening anatomically or physiologically to these
understimulated children, might we not then be able objec-
tively and realistically to plan a 'better' world for them?
   Until recently, nothing was known about the mechanisms
of deprivation, though much, of course, about the negative
results. At long last, however, we are beginning to understand
rather precisely what a 'poor' environment does, or rather
does not do, for the mammalian organisms inhabiting it. In
an article in the February 1972 issue of Scientific American
entitled 'Brain Changes in Response to Experience', Mark
Rosenzweig and his associates at the University of California
in Berkeley summarized a series of experiments which
demonstrated that baby rats raised in large cages with a
diversity of stimulating objects turned out to become much
more adept at solving problems than their siblings kept in
smaller, duller cages.
    A 'better' environment produced 'better' rats - of course!
That in itself any biology teacher, in fact, almost any man in
the street could have predicted. But Rosenzweig and his
associates were able for the first time to show clearly in
morphilogical, anatomical and physiological terms why these
animals grow up to be more intelligent. By measuring,
sectioning, and comparing their brains, they demonstrated a
direct relationship between environmental diversity and the
size and probable complexity of the brain (cf. Henderson
1970). In a stimulating environment, the cerebral cortex of the
brains of baby rats grew larger by about 5 per cent to 10 per
cent compared to those kept in a dull environment, a truly sig-
nificant increase! Or to put it another way, the ordinary and



304
dull rat environment produced brains with cerebral cortices
which were below normal by about 5 per cent. In effect, only
in a rich or stimulating environment can the brains of baby rats
reach their normal potential. Not only was brain size, brain
weight, cortex thickness, etc. increased, but the individual
nerve cells were shown to have grown more complex as a result
of stimulation by diversity: with dendrites more branched and
spines more abundant. Briefly, in the stimulating environment
the nerve cells of the rat babies would 'fire' more often. This
in turn somehow would cause them to grow more, to become
more complex. Analogous to the enlarged biceps of the lumber-
jack, the brain 'adapts' by enlarging; or, again, to restate this
in perhaps a more accurate way, the dull environment
prevents the development of the highest or most optimal
neural potentialities which, in a more natural environment,
would ordinarily have been reached as a matter of course.
This restatement makes it clear that, for baby rats, at least,
diversity is not a luxury but biologically the pre-condition for
normal development. Only a stimulated brain can become a
fully developed and complicated (i. e. normal) brain!
Demonstrated experimentally only for rats, it is probably an
adaptation of all mammals.
   If true for rat babies, is such stimulatory enrichment of
brain potential (or suppression of potential) also true for
human babies? That question, of course, cannot be answered
with anatomical evidence, for such experiments with human
babies cannot be done since mothers and fathers would
object to having their offspring frozen, killed, and micro-
tomed, even for a good cause! By analogy, however, (and
perhaps by homology, too) what these tremendously signi-
ficant studies clearly say is this: give a growing young
mammal, from the earliest months on, a stimulating and
healthy, more natural environment, and it will grow up to
fulfill its innate potentialities, and develop a more complex,
better and probably more harmoniously functioning and
larger brain, and, perhaps, a healthier body and psyche as
well. The animal will, in fact, become more intelligent!
Applied to humans, it means brighter children, happier
adults.
   The cultural (as well as neurological) gap between
humanity and other mammals, including rats, is of course

                                                                                          305
immense. Rats, for example, are more nocturnal, with a more
developed sense of smell and touch, humans more diurnal,
with a much more developed sense of vision, natural pattern
and sound. Because of our evolutionary histories, our needs
differ in the kinds of stimulation we might need. Compared
to all other mammals, Man does stand alone, unique in a
hundred ways - the walking, talking, writing, reading, car-
driving, pair-bonding, fire-making, toilet-trained, social naked
ape. Yet, even if we are very human, we still share all of our
basic physical and psychological attributes with the other
primates, and nursing and caring for the young with all other
mammals. Thus, as remarkable as our species is, we must not
continue to be blinded by our own self-image into believing
we are at heart not animals with deep and innate needs and
cravings which command so many of our actions. Therefore,
the results of animal experiments are meaningful to teachers,
to planners, and to politicians. Indeed, our basic human
patterns, both physiologically and behaviourally, and those
of the primates hardly differ at all. One has only to read Jane
Van Lawick-Goodall's (1971) account of her life with a tribe
of chimpanzees in Africa to realise that both happiness and
misery have universal roots in all the anthropoid apes,
including man. Especially for the young in their early
ontogeny, happiness means satisfying basic innate natural
needs - and to babies the cultural satisfactions, which so
many intellectualized adults insist on, are just so much extra
whipped cream.

The evolution of intelligence as
a function of diversity
In 'A Biologic View of Human History', a review of
Seidenberg's (1950) memorable Posthistoric Man, the gene-
ticist Bebtley Glass (1951) says:

   The intelligence of man is an evolutionary product of
    natural selection for adaptability to great variation of
   surroundings, to tremendous vicissitudes of experience, as
   Bergson concluded forty years ago. It should follow from
   the nature of natural selection that the prolongation of the
   dominance of intelligence in man must depend upon the
   maintenance of the variety and richness, the vast complexity



306
   of that world to which he must adjust his ways.

In the evolution of the human species, the vast diversity both
in immediate surroundings and in personal experience pro-
duced increasingly higher levels of intelligence through
natural selection, which was applied to cope with the
multiplicity of choices which such a diversity not only offers,
but imposes. Likewise, the potential for an increase in brain
complexity in response to diversity (especially during the
early life stages of a mammal, as shown by the above cited
experiments of Rosenzweig) may be viewed as a genetic
adaptation with survival value for those individuals which
find themselves, for whatever reason (and these in turn
probably largely based on genetic factors also), in a diverse
environment (i. e. more productive habitat) and which will
thus produce more offspring, and then brighter ones, than
those which settled in a dull, less productive environment.
   Today, more than ever, it is evident that mankind needs
diversity and its stimulation not only to elicit the
anatomical-neurological responses of the individual growing
brain in childhood, bur, on the global and phyletic scale, to
continue the high intelligence of the human species. As Glass
points out in the above review, just as environmental
diversity elicited the evolution of high intelligence in Homo
sapiens, so the general lack of diversity will most likely result
in a degeneration of this intelligence. The eyes of cave fish,
the legs of snakes and the useless muscles in the human ear
are cited as telling examples. It is a truism that, to maintain
any biological attribute, we need the continuation of the
environmental selective factors which produced it. We need
the stimulation of diversity to maintain high human intelli-
gence, on both the personal and on the phyletic (species)
level, and, continues Glass, 'given an environment of diversity
and complexity, the evolution of intelligence should pro-
ceed'.
   Yet what kinds of diversities? I realise that there are all
sorts, and civilization produces its own unique brands. But
especially compelling and significant for man, I feel, must be
the incredibly rich diversity of the 400,000 species of plants
and four million or more species of animals which combine
and recombine endlessly into a vast array of perpetually and

                                                                                      307
astronomically variable biotic communities. From all indi-
cations, this visual natural diversity is especially meaningful
to man, and here, at the earliest ontogenetic stages, almost
compulsively searched for by children. It is well to stand in
awe before this variability, which is of a magnitude quite
irreproducible by technology.
   Will mankind have enough sense to preserve the com-
plexity and diversity of nature which may be necessary to
induce, and to continue or preserve, high human intelligence?
The current rate of heedless homogenation of the world's
once diverse environments is not encouraging. The transfor-
mation of natural diversity into cornfields, cow pastures and
concrete cities on a worldwide scale appears almost in-
evitable. Yet, concurrently, the marvellously interesting
cultural diversity of the world is being obliterated as well.
Eventually, human intelligence might well degenerate into
stereotyped responses to the few stimuli allowed to survive.
   Perhaps this evolutionary repression is already beginning in
our larger cities where current unnatural selection pressures
must be not only totally different from what they (until
recently) were in more natural and benign environments, but
are probably favouring human genotypes immune to ugliness,
noise and pollution, and insensitive towards the life-
sustaining systems of nature, since there, in a Chicago or a
New York, these have all but ceased to exist. That is not to
say that cities do not have compensating good qualities,
especially for the socializing adolescent. But pity the young
child who needs to see his first frog. Or, indeed, any sensitive
or timid human in need of a slower pace, a more tranquil,
natural way of living.
   Continued 'growth' and crowding of course will inevitably
increase these pressures. Growth both of population and of
technology implies bigger and grimmer megalopolises and, if
continued, condemns future generations (especially of the
inner-city poor and black) to become permanently locked
into permanently denatured and irrevocably polluted
environments. The promise of an unlimited energy supply,
especially, is the grimmest prospect of all; for it would
postpone almost immediately any population curbs, increase
tremendously industrial development and the 'bloating' of
cities into megalopolises, and ensure the utter destruction of



308
nature, and with it, ultimately, not only the source of our
food and fibre but the sensitive and joyful nature of man.
   If we value human intelligence, its superb sensitivities and
deep emotions towards nature, and the happiness which ]
flows from it, we had better value the natural diversity which
still happens to exist by default or accident, and deliberately
preserve it. Thus, by whatever route we approach the modern
human problem, the answer is always the same: preserve
nature, for it is sacred to humanity and its evolution.
  
Role of biology teachers
Why do people love flowers and shrubs more than rusty tin
cans? Why do people insist on planting flower boxes and
tilling vegetable and rose gardens even in the poorest sections
of a town, often against impossible odds, and hire landscape
architects at great expense in rich suburbs? Lead poisoning
aside, why are there so many school problems with the
deprived children of the poor, especially blacks, in big
city ghettos? And, finally, to beg a question, why must we
now, at this late date, and at the last chance man will ever
have, preserve the biotic richness of the lovely and good
earth? Only through modern research may we arrive at
definitive answers to these difficult and politically loaded
questions.
   How must biology teachers relate to the fantastic new
discoveries of science? How could biology teachers rationally
apply this increased understanding of the nature of man to
their everyday activities?
   Biology teachers could do much to become a major force
for ecological good sense. They, more than other teachers,
should have the knowledge of scientific principles on which
this good sense must be based. They are capable of
comprehending and accepting the fact that the animal origins
of humanity are at its very base. They can appreciate the
need for modern ethological and physiological research and
be critical of the vagueness of many current precepts. They
are aware that man is a slowly evolved and still slowly
evolving primate species, and that any consideration of man
in relation to his environment should be placed within the
framework of his evolutionary nature. Biology teachers,
especially, should be aware of the need for multidisciplinary
                                                                                   309
studies of man, studies which must consider his original
environment antedating the industrial and medical revolu-
tion, including the ethological predispositions of his tribal
pre-nuclear family (cf. Greenbie 1972), the slowness of
human evolution, the nature of human natural selection, as
well as the great subtleties of chemical, physical and visual
pollution effects  and their relationships to sensory and
psychological deprivation (Randolph 1962; Cassidy 1967;
Iltis 1973a)
   Many biology teachers, however, will need to undergo
fundamental changes of attitudes; for the biology teacher as
an intellectual revolutionary is not the image he generally
projects. To encourage such rethinking and redoing, here are
a few suggestions for action:

1.  Above all, read critically the writings of the many
biologists, environmentalists and social thinkers, such as Paul
and Anne Ehrlich's Population, Resources and Environment,
Barry Commoner's The Closing Circle, Meadows et al., The
Limits to Growth, Garrett Hardin's Population, Evolution and
Birth Control, Shepard and McKinley's The Subversive
Science and Environ/Mental, or even Rachel Carson's Silent
Spring. To get some idea of the limitations to man's
technological use of this Earth, the report by the US National
Academy of Science - National Research Council's  Commit-
tee on Resources and Man should convince even the most
optimistic whither our civilization is drifting. The journal
Environment in America, The Ecologist in Great Britain, as
well as those of other environmental organizations belong in
every biology teacher's home. Three exceedingly useful
bibliographies also need to be mentioned, On Rediscovering
the Biosphere by Dan McKinley (1970), Science for Society,
edited by John Moore (1971) and The Environmental Crisis:
A Bibliography by Carol Sherr (1973).

2.  Read the writings of the new school of evolutionary
perceptive, ethologically oriented anthropologists: the many
sensitive books of Ashley Montague, Desmond Morris's The
Human Zoo and Intimate Behaviour, Robert Ardrey's
African Genesis and The Territorial Imperative, Anthony
Storr's Human Aggression, Eibl-Eiblesfield's Ethology, Tiger
and Fox's The Imperial Animal, Edward T. Hall's The Hidden



310
Dimension. Paul Shepard's masterful The Tender Carnivore
and the Sacred Game, works by Sommer, Lorenz, Tinbergen,
Elton, and even Rene Spitz's The First Year of Life. Not that
these authors will agree with each other, far from it. But you
may get a feeling for the immense force of the primitive
genetic components which interact with culture, and
especially for the sensitive innate needs of the human animal
during childhood. For the lower level teacher, the short but
excellent Basic Ecology text of the Buchsbaums (1957),
written clairvoyantly before the environmental crisis, is still
to be highly recommended. Modern textbooks dealing with
the general subject of human ecology and evolution will be
most rewarding (e. g. Odum 1971; Boughey 1972; Ehrlich et
al., 1973) but, in a sense, every book listed so far deals with
these topics.
   It is crucial to realise that most of the authors here listed
ignore or underplay human genetic needs for nature, or at
best tend to confuse it with cultural sophistications of the
elite. Very few have a holistic view of genetic programming,
cultural training, and environmental input as an indivisible
unity (cf. Bateson, Steps to an Ecology of Mind). And very
few indeed are truly concerned with, or even aware of, the
most irreplaceable and irreversible, tragic, and, yet, ultimately
most stupid effect of the exponential growth of human
activity, population, and affluence: the current extinction of
plant and animal species and ecosystems on a gigantic global
scale, especially now in the tropical rain-forest regions, and
soon in all the deserts and oceans. David Ehrenfield's superb
book, Preserving Life on Earth, Dasmann's A Different Kind
of Country, Dorst's Before Nature Dies, the beautiful Wildlife
in Danger by Fisher, Simon and Vincent, the many publica-
tions of the International Union for Conservation of Nature
and Natural Resources (IUCN), including its excellent
bulletin, and some of my own papers speak to that point. But
it is a little hard to comprehend why the otherwise valuable
semi-official guide book for the United Nations Stockholm
Conference on the Human Environment, Only One Earth, by
Barbara Ward and Rene Dubos, hardly mentions extinction
of species or of diversity, an almost inconceivable yet not
really surprising oversight, considering that most authors and
readers today are city-born and city-bred. Even Konrad


                                                                                          311
Lorenz' latest book, on the eight deadly sins of modern man,
singles out growth, overpopulation and pollution, but the
ruthless eradication of ecosystems and the resulting extinc-
tion of plants and animal species is not one of them. Why are
we, even renowned biologists, so reluctant to mourn the loss
of species, this incredible, incalculable and, from most any
perspective, insane calamity?

3.   The need for nature is yet to be objectively documented.
There are a multitude of subjective indications, from religion,
art, and music to gardening and psychiatry, that suggest man
to be indivisible from the living natural environments around
him. On the other hand, highly objective indications, such as
the physiological-anatomical work cited above, are rare
indeed. Clearly, more information, more research is des-
perately needed on man's perception of the whole living,
blooming, fruiting plant, and animal, both in the landscape
and in his kitchen, before it is too late. While Plants/People/
and Environmental Quality (Robinette 1972) has little to say
on man's genetic needs for plants or nature,  it does deal
thoroughly with the architectural, engineering and climato-
logical uses of plants, and includes a very extensive biblio-
graphy on that aspect. Paul Shephard's Man in the Landscape
is one of the few books that delves deeply into our
fundamental physical and psychological adaptations to
nature. There is now a vast array of books, symposia and
anthologies dealing with the environmental crisis, many of
which will give isolated perceptive insights into this question
(e. g. the preface to Fisher et al. )
   Lewis Mumford's The Myth of the Machine, and others of
his books enable one to see technology not only as a servant
but as a master of humanity. It might not be amiss to point
out that there is now a compendium of failures of tech-
nology. On the large scale, and in the long run, it just doesn't
work! The Careless Technology, edited by Farvar and Milton,
'is a book full of failures, and failures of the most vivid sort,
in fields that are at the heart of what we think we know most
about: building dams, fighting disease, making food available,
etc. It is the Green Revolution a dozen times over', (Dan
McKinley, personal comment). Paul Goodman's essay Can
Technology be Humane? insists on prudence and on the



312
need, not only for a change in the values and organization of
science, but 'in the kinds of men who make scientific
decisions'. Roderick Seidenberg's difficult Posthistoric Man is
hardly optimistic about the outcome of human evolution,
especially if we do not consciously take alternatives to a
technology which makes human activities, and even human
needs, superfluous. Rene Dubos's A God Within, Man
Adapting and So Human an Animal contain valuable bio-
logical observation of man's intimate responses to nature, yet
here, and especially in many recent papers Dubos (1973) has
turned into a kind of environmental 'Uncle Tom', taking
potshots at those concerned with the preservation of species
and ecosystems. In some of my papers, I have cited Maxwell
Weissman (1965), a psychiatrist, who found much improve-
ment in mentally ill patients being taken to a rural natural
setting. Others have made similar observations without
follow-up research. Our lack of hard knowledge on nature as
a healing environment is but a reflection of our almost
deliberate and cynical neglect of studying the man-nature
relationship, despite the fullness of our mental hospitals and
the many other pathological manifestations of a denatured
and sick world.

4.   Each biology teacher is on his own to comprehend the
evolutionary nature of mankind and synthesise his own
philosophy. Hopefully, this will be based on prudence,
restraint, respect for innate needs, and a sincere desire to
reduce the impact of humanity on the earth. Perhaps nothing
is more important today than this. We as individual teachers
have to develop our own biological sophistication, or we are
lost. What is one to think of pontifical projections of an
optimum (!) world population of 50 billion (the figure given
by some Russian planners, by the Australian Colin Clarke and
the Greek planner Doxiadis), 36 billion (the FAO), or even a
mere nine billion (by none other, of all people, than the very
man who showed what crowding did to rats; cf Calhoun,
1968), when today, the world in 1973 is heading into
unprecedented famine with not quite four billion, and when
in fact a total world population of one billion would be more
than an ample breeding population and be capable of doing
anything the human species would ever want to do? How is
                                                                                        313
one to deal with Krieger's What's Wrong with Plastic Trees?
or any of the legion of Utopian environmental optimists
which lately have found such favourable editorial reception in
Science and Nature, except by ridicule ? (Iltis 1973b). It is
obviously important to examine critically the 'glad tidings' of
these optimists who will not accept the premise that a finite
world has finite resources, including plants and animals; who
readily promise, even today, the pie-in-the-sky of unlimited
resources, unlimited food, unlimited oceans, unlimited
atomic energy, unlimited population, and unlimited wealth,
but who neglect to mention the unlimited misery humanity is
heading into as a consequence, or the unlimited extinction of
plant and animal species. Newspapers and magazines, like-
wise, abound with articles making self-fulfilling prophesies:
the 'need' for more growth, more electric power, more food,
more roads, and more of nearly everything. It is frustrating
and galling to observe that in this 'scientific' age, everyone
seems to be culturally conditioned to demand exacting proof
of human 'needs' for nature, while no one ever questions the
'need' for more cars, more cows, or more concrete, needs
that in no sense are biological.
   Remember, nobody ever can get something for nothing.
'There is no such thing as a free lunch' to quote Barry
Commoner's '4th Law' of ecology. Atomic power without
polluting radiation; Green Revolutions without depletion of
genetic diversity, without pesticides, or without disastrous
plant diseases; population expansion without extinction of
plants and animal species; these are the pipe dreams out of
which the ultimate human tragedy is now being deliberately
constructed. Thus, it is true, as a brilliant if sick joke will
have it: 'If you want a higher standard of living, you will have
to settle for a lower quality of life!'
   Finally, if an all-encompassing eco-evolution is indeed on
the horizon (and it had better be if we are to survive with any
sanity), it must be based on a thoroughly ecologically-sound
restructuring of society to ensure the survival of biotic
diversity. The human species, once free and frivolous in its
habits of consumption, must be brought into a steady-state
harmony with the natural environment. The limitation of
population size and consequent restructuring of economics
and priorities is the fundamental prerequisite for a



314
meaningful long-term  resolution of the environmental crisis.
The ultimate aim for our species must be to find the basic
optimum human environment: a compromise between our
cultural accomplishments and our biological needs - a com-
promise between many of the advantages of technological
urban civilization, which have freed man's hands from labour
and man's brains for creativity and which we are loath to give
up; and the innate and often subtle human needs for the
evolutionary theatre of nature, the wild environment which
produced our bodies and brains over millions of years
through natural selection; needs which we cannot ignore
except at gravest peril to our health, sanity, and happiness.
   The world is changing at a terrifying rate, but human needs
for nature will remain essentially the same, for millenia and
decimillenia to come. Even without knowing the precise
physiological or neurological details on which these needs are
based, we must respect them as the forces which have
moulded our species both physically and psychologically. It
shall be the true role of biology to make our life livable and
human by insisting that our inherited needs for natural
pattern, natural form, and natural diversity are not aban-
doned wherever we may be in favour of a capricious
technological hell. In the preservation of nature lies the
salvation not only of biology teaching, but of humanity as
well.













References

R. Ardrey (1961) African Genesis, Collins.
R. Ardrey (1967) The Territorial Imperative, Collins.
R. Carson (1965) Silent Spring, Penguin.\
B. Commoner (1972) The Closing Circle, Cape.
R. F. Dasmann (1968) A Different Kind of Country, Macmillan,
      New York.
J. Dorst (1970) Before Nature Dies, Collins.
R. Dubos (1965) Man Adapting, Yale University Press.
R. Dubos (1970) So Human an Animal, Hart-Davis.

                                                                                                    315
R. Dubos (1972) A God Within, Scribner's.
R. Dubos (1973) 'Humanizing Earth', Science, 179.
D. Ehrenfeld (1972) Preserving Life on Earth, Oxford University Press,
     New York.
P.R. and A. H. Ehrlich. (1972) Population, Resources and Environ-
     ment: Issues in Human Ecology, 2nd edition, W. H. Freeman.
M. T. Farvar and J. P. Milton (eds.) (1973) The Careless Technology,
    Tom Stacey.
J. Fisher, N. Simon and J. Vincent (1969) Wildlife in Danger, Viking
    Press.
P. Goodman (1969) 'Can Technology be Humane?', New York Review
    of Books, 20 November, 1969, reprinted in The Social Responsi-
    bility of the Scientist, edited by Martin Brown, 1971, Free Press.
T. Hall (1966) The Hidden Dimension, Doubleday.
G. J. Hardin (ed.) (1964) Population, Evolution and Birth Control: a
    Collage of Controversial Ideas, W. H. Freeman.
M. H. Krieger (1973) 'What's wrong with Plastic Trees?', Science, 179
    (4072). An edited version 'Up the Plastic Tree' reprinted in
    Landscape Architecture, 63 (4) (1973).
S. Langer (1967) Mind: An Essay on Human Feeling, vol. 1, Johns
    Hopkins University Presss.
A. Leopold (1949) A Sand County Almanac, Oxford University Press,
     New York.
K. Lorenz and P. Leyhausen (1973) Motivation of Human and Animal
     Behaviour, Van Nostrand Reinhold.
D. McKinley (1970) 'On Rediscovering the Biosphere', The Explorer,
     12 (2).
D. H. and D. L. Meadows, J. Randers and W. W. Behrens (1972) The
    Limits to Growth, Universe Books.
J. A. Moore (ed.) (1971) Science for Society: a Bibliography, revised
    edition, American Association for the Advancement of Science.
D. Morris (1969) The Human Zoo, Cape.
D. Morris (1971) Intimate Behaviour, Cape.
L. Mumford, (1967) The Myth of the Machine, vol. 1, Technics and
    Huan Development, Secker & Warburg.
L. Mumford (1971) The Myth of the Machine, vol. 2, The Pentagon of
    Power, Secker & Warburg.
G. O. Robinette (1972) Plants/People/and Environmental Quality, a
    study of Plants and their Environmental Functions, U. S. Govern-
    ment Printing Office.
M. R. Rosenzweig, E. L. Bennett and M. C. Diamond (1972) 'Brain
    Changes in Response to Experience', Scientific American 226 (2).
R. Seidenberg (1950) Posthistoric Man: an Inquiry, University of
    North Carolina Press, reprinted in 1957 by Beacon Press.
P. Shephard (1967) Man in the Landscape: a Historic View of the
    Esthetics of Nature, Knopf.
P. Shephard (1973) The Tender Carnivore and the Sacred Game,
    Scribner's.
C. Sherr (1973) The Environmental Crisis: a Bibliography in  Lange, C.




316
     T. and Klinge, P. E. , see Further Reading.
R. Spitz (1965) The First Year of Life, International University Press.
C. W. Stillman (1972) 'Reflections on Environmental Education',
    Teachers College Record, 74 (2).
A. Storr (1969) Human Agression, Allen Lane.
J. Van Lawick-Goodall (1971) In the Shadow of Man, Houghton
    Mifflin.
B. Ward and R. Dubos (1972) Only One Earth: the Care and
    Maintenance of a Small Planet, Penguin.

Further Reading

G. Bateson (1973) Steps to an Ecology of Mind, Ballantine.
A. S. Boughey (1971) Man and Environment: an Introduction to
    Human Ecology and Evolution, Macmillan, New York.
M. and R. Buchsbaum (1957) Basic Ecology, Boxwood Press.
J. B. Calhoun (1968) 'Space and the Strategy of Life', paper presented
    at The American Association for the Advancement of Science,
    Dallas.
H. G. Cassidy (1967) 'On Incipient Environmental Collapse',
    BioScience, 17.
R. M. Chute (1971) Environmental Insight: Readings and Comment
     on
Human and Nonhuman Nature, Harper and Row.
C. A. Doxiadis (1966) Urban Renewal and the Future of The
    American City, Public Administration Service, Chicago.
P. R. and A. H. Ehrlich and J. P. Holdren (1973) Human Ecology:
    Problems and Solutions, W. H. Freeman.
P. Ehrlich and J. Freedman (1971) 'Population, Crowding and Human
    Behavior', New Scientist and Science Journal, 1 April, 1971.
I. Eibl-Eibelsfeldt (1970) Ethology, Holt, Reinhart and Winston.
Environment, vols. 1-15 (1973), P.O. Box 755, Bridgeton, Missouri
    63044
J.W. Forester (1971) World Dynamics, Wright-Allen Press.
J. Fredman, P. Ehrlich and S. Klevansky (1971) 'The Effect of
    Crowding on Human Task Performance', Journal of Applied Social
   Psychology, 1 (1).
B. Glass (1951) 'A Biologic View of Human History', Scientific
    American, December (review of Seidenberg's Posthistoric Man)
B. Greenbie (1972) Homo Sapiens Habitat: Implications of Ethology
    for the Design and Planning of Human Habitations. PhD thesis,
    University of Wisconsin.
N. D. Henderson (1970) 'Brain Weight Increases Resulting from
    Environmental Enrichment: A directional Dominance in Mice',
    Science 169.
J. P. Holdren and P. Ehrlich (eds.) (1971) Global Ecology: Readings
    Toward a National Strategy for Man, Harcourt Brace.
H. H. Iltis (1959) 'We Need Many More Scientific Areas', Wisconsin
    Conservation Bulletin, 24 (9).
H. H. Iltis (1966) 'The Meaning of Human Evolution to Conservation',

                                                                                               317
    Wisconsin Academy Review 13 (2).
H. H. Iltis (1967) 'To the Taxonimist and Zoologist: Whose Fight is
    the Preservation of Nature?' BioScience 17 (12).
H. H. Iltis (1968) 'The Optimum Human Environment and its
    Relation to Modern Agricultural Preoccupations', The Biologist
    50 (3-4).
H. H. Iltis (1972) 'Conservation, Contraception and Catholicism, A
    20th Century Trinity', The Biologist 54 (1).
H. H. Iltis (1972) 'The Biology Teacher and Man's Mad and Final
    War on Nature', The American Biology Teacher 34 (3).
H. H. Iltis (1972) 'The Extinction of Species and the Destruction of
    Ecosystems', The American Biology Teacher 34 (4) April.
H. H. Iltis (1973a) 'Pollution and Adaptation: What Hope for Man' in
    C. T. Lange and P. E. Klinge (eds.) Pollution, 1-6, (see below).
H. H. Iltis (1973b) 'Down the Technological Fix', (original title: 'Can
    One Love a Plastic Tree?', an answer to Krieger 1973),
    Landscape Architecture 63 (4).
H. H. Iltis, O. L. Loucks and P. Andrews (1970) 'Criteria for an
    Optium Human Environment', Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists
    25 (1).
C. T. Lange and P. E. Klinge (1973) Pollution, A special publication
    of the National Association of Biology Teachers, 1420 N. Street,
    N. W. , Washington, D. C. , 20005
National Academy of Science- National Research Council (1969)
    Resources and Man, W. H. Freeman.
H. T. Odum (1971) Environment, Power and Society, Wiley-
    Interscience.
E. P. Odum (1971) Fundamentals of Ecology, 3rd Edition, W. B.
    Saunders.
T. Randolph (1962) Human Ecology and Sensitivity to the
     Chemical
Environment, Charles C. Thomas.
P. Shephard and D. McKinley (eds.) (1969) The Subversive Science:
    Essays towards and Ecology of Man, Houghton-Mifflin.
P. Shephard and D. McKinley (eds.) (1971) Environ/Mental: Essays
    on the Planet as a Home, Houghton-Mifflin.
L. Tiger and R. Fox (1972) The Imperial Animal, Secker and
     Warburg.
The Ecologist : 'Man and the Environment', 'The Quality of Life',
    'Pollution', 'Conservation', vol. 1 (1971) from 73 Kew Green,
    Richmond, Surrey.
E. A. and N. Tinbergen (1972) Early Childhood Autism - An
    Ethological Approach, Fortschritte der Verhaltensforschung,
    Beihefte zur Zeitschrift fur Tier psychologie 10 (Advances in
    Ethology, Supplement to Journal of Comparative Ethology,
    10, Verlag Paul Perey, Berlin und Hamburg.)